Writing the Empirical Journal Article
Daryl J. Bem
Cornell University
Planning Your Article
.
2
Which Article Should You Write? 2
Analyzing Data 2
Reporting the Findings 2
How Should You Write? 3
For Whom Should You Write? 3
Writing Your Article 4
The Shape of An Article 4
The Introduction 4
The Opening Statements 4
Examples of Examples 5
The Literature Review 5
Citations 6
Criticizing Previous Work 6
Ending the Introduction 6
The Method Section 6
The Results Section 7
Setting the Stage 7
Presenting the Findings 8
Figures and Tables 9
On Statistics 9
The Discussion Section 9
The Title and Abstract 10
Rewriting and Polishing Your Article 11
Some Matters of Style 12
Omit Needless Words 12
Avoid Metacomments on the Writing 13
Use Repetition and Parallel Construction 13
Jargon 14
Voice and Self-Reference 14
Tense 14
Avoid Language Bias 14
Research Participants 14
Sex and Gender 14
Racial and Ethnic Identity 15
Sexual Orientation 15
Disabilities 16
Common Errors of Grammar and Usage 16
Compared with versus Compared to 16
Data 16
Different from versus Different than 16
Since versus Because 16
That versus Which 16
While versus Although, But, Whereas 16
Publishing Your Article 16
References 17
A version of this article appears in Darley, J. M., Zanna, M. P., & Roediger III, H. L. (Eds) (2003). The Compleat Academic:
A Practical Guide for the Beginning Social Scientist, 2nd Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 2
You have conducted a study and analyzed the data.
Now it is time to write. To publish. To tell the world what
you have learned. The purpose of this article is to enhance
the chances that some journal editor will let you do so.
If you are new to this enterprise, you may find it
helpful to consult two additional sources of information.
For detailed information on the proper format of a journal
article, see the Publication Manual of the American Psy-
chological Association (APA, 2001) and recent articles in
the journal to which you plan to submit your manuscript.
For renewing your acquaintance with the formal and sty-
listic elements of English prose, you can read Chapter 2
of the Publication Manual or any one of several style
manuals. I recommend The Elements of Style by Strunk
and White (2000). It is brief, witty, and inexpensive.
Because I write, review, and edit primarily for jour-
nals in personality and social psychology, I have drawn
most of my examples from those areas. Colleagues assure
me, however, that the guidelines set forth here are also
pertinent for articles in experimental psychology and bi-
opsychology. Similarly, this article focuses on the report
of an empirical study, but the general writing suggestions
apply as well to the theoretical articles, literature reviews,
and methodological contributions that also appear in our
journals. (Specific guidance for preparing a literature re-
view article for Psychological Bulletin can be found in
Bem, 1995.)
Planning Your Article
Which Article Should You Write?
There are two possible articles you can write: (a) the
article you planned to write when you designed your
study or (b) the article that makes the most sense now that
you have seen the results. They are rarely the same, and
the correct answer is (b).
The conventional view of the research process is that
we first derive a set of hypotheses from a theory, design
and conduct a study to test these hypotheses, analyze the
data to see if they were confirmed or disconfirmed, and
then chronicle this sequence of events in the journal arti-
cle. If this is how our enterprise actually proceeded, we
could write most of the article before we collected the
data. We could write the introduction and method sections
completely, prepare the results section in skeleton form,
leaving spaces to be filled in by the specific numerical
results, and have two possible discussion sections ready to
go, one for positive results, the other for negative results.
But this is not how our enterprise actually proceeds.
Psychology is more exciting than that, and the best jour-
nal articles are informed by the actual empirical findings
from the opening sentence. Before writing your article,
then, you need to Analyze Your Data. Herewith, a ser-
monette on the topic.
Analyzing Data. Once upon a time, psychologists ob-
served behavior directly, often for sustained periods of
time. No longer. Now, the higher the investigator goes up
the tenure ladder, the more remote he or she typically
becomes from the grounding observations of our science.
If you are already a successful research psychologist, then
you probably haven’t seen a participant for some time.
Your graduate assistant assigns the running of a study to a
bright undergraduate who writes the computer program
that collects the data automatically. And like the modern
dentist, the modern psychologist rarely even sees the data
until they have been cleaned by human or computer hy-
gienists.
To compensate for this remoteness from our partici-
pants, let us at least become intimately familiar with the
record of their behavior: the data. Examine them from
every angle. Analyze the sexes separately. Make up new
composite indexes. If a datum suggests a new hypothesis,
try to find additional evidence for it elsewhere in the data.
If you see dim traces of interesting patterns, try to reor-
ganize the data to bring them into bolder relief. If there
are participants you don’t like, or trials, observers, or in-
terviewers who gave you anomalous results, drop them
(temporarily). Go on a fishing expedition for some-
thing—anything —interesting.
No, this is not immoral. The rules of scientific and
statistical inference that we overlearn in graduate school
apply to the “Context of Justification.” They tell us what
we can conclude in the articles we write for public con-
sumption, and they give our readers criteria for deciding
whether or not to believe us. But in the “Context of Dis-
covery,” there are no formal rules, only heuristics or
strategies. How does one discover a new phenomenon?
Smell a good idea? Have a brilliant insight into behavior?
Create a new theory? In the confining context of an em-
pirical study, there is only one strategy for discovery:
exploring the data.
Yes, there is a danger. Spurious findings can emerge
by chance, and we need to be cautious about anything we
discover in this way. In limited cases, there are statistical
techniques that correct for this danger. But there are no
statistical correctives for overlooking an important dis-
covery because we were insufficiently attentive to the
data. Let us err on the side of discovery.
Reporting the Findings. When you are through ex-
ploring, you may conclude that the data are not strong
enough to justify your new insights formally, but at least
you are now ready to design the “right” study. If you still
plan to report the current data, you may wish to mention
the new insights tentatively, stating honestly that they
remain to be tested adequately. Alternatively, the data
may be strong enough to justify recentering your article
around the new findings and subordinating or even ig-
noring your original hypotheses.
This is not advice to suppress negative results. If your
study was genuinely designed to test hypotheses that de-
rive from a formal theory or are of wide general interest
for some other reason, then they should remain the focus
of your article. The integrity of the scientific enterprise
requires the reporting of disconfirming results.
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 3
But this requirement assumes that somebody out
there cares about the hypotheses. Many respectable stud-
ies are explicitly exploratory or are launched from specu-
lations of the “I-wonder-if ...” variety. If your study is one
of these, then nobody cares if you were wrong. Contrary
to the conventional wisdom, science does not care how
clever or clairvoyant you were at guessing your results
ahead of time. Scientific integrity does not require you to
lead your readers through all your wrongheaded hunches
only to show— voila!—they were wrongheaded. A jour-
nal article should not be a personal history of your still-
born thoughts.
Your overriding purpose is to tell the world what you
have learned from your study. If your results suggest a
compelling framework for their presentation, adopt it and
make the most instructive findings your centerpiece.
Think of your dataset as a jewel. Your task is to cut and
polish it, to select the facets to highlight, and to craft the
best setting for it. Many experienced authors write the
results section first.
But before writing anything, Analyze Your Data!
End of sermonette.
How Should You Write?
The primary criteria for good scientific writing are
accuracy and clarity. If your article is interesting and
written with style, fine. But these are subsidiary virtues.
First strive for accuracy and clarity.
The first step toward clarity is good organization, and
the standardized format of a journal article does much of
the work for you. It not only permits readers to read the
report from beginning to end, as they would any coherent
narrative, but also to scan it for a quick overview of the
study or to locate specific information easily by turning
directly to the relevant section. Within that format, how-
ever, it is still helpful to work from an outline of your
own. This enables you to examine the logic of the se-
quence, to spot important points that are omitted or mis-
placed, and to decide how best to divide the labor of pres-
entation between the introduction and final discussion
(about which, more later).
The second step toward clarity is to write simply and
directly. A journal article tells a straightforward tale of a
circumscribed problem in search of a solution. It is not a
novel with subplots, flashbacks, and literary allusions, but
a short story with a single linear narrative line. Let this
line stand out in bold relief. Don’t make your voice strug-
gle to be heard above the ambient noise of cluttered writ-
ing. You are justifiably proud of your 90th percentile ver-
bal aptitude, but let it nourish your prose, not glut it.
Write simply and directly.
For Whom Should You Write?
Scientific journals are published for specialized audi-
ences who share a common background of substantive
knowledge and methodological expertise. If you wish to
write well, you should ignore this fact. Psychology en-
compasses a broader range of topics and methodologies
than do most other disciplines, and its findings are fre-
quently of interest to a wider public. The social psycholo-
gist should be able to read a Psychometrika article on
logistic analysis; the personality theorist, a biopsychology
article on hypothalamic function; and the congressional
aide with a BA in history, a Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology article on causal attribution.
Accordingly, good writing is good teaching. Direct
your writing to the student in Psychology 101, your col-
league in the Art History Department, and your grand-
mother. No matter how technical or abstruse your article
is in its particulars, intelligent nonpsychologists with no
expertise in statistics or experimental design should be
able to comprehend the broad outlines of what you did
and why. They should understand in general terms what
was learned. And above all, they should appreciate why
someone—anyone—should give a damn. The introduc-
tion and discussion sections in particular should be acces-
sible to this wider audience.
The actual technical materials—those found primar-
ily in the method and results sections—should be aimed at
a reader one level of expertise less specialized than the
audience for which the journal is primarily published.
Assume that the reader of your article in Psychometrika
knows about regression, but needs some introduction to
logistic analysis. Assume that the reader of the Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology knows about person
perception but needs some introduction to dispositional
and situational attributions.
Many of the writing techniques suggested in this arti-
cle are thus teaching techniques designed to make your
article comprehensible to the widest possible audience.
They are also designed to remain invisible or transparent
to your readers, thereby infusing your prose with a “sub-
liminal pedagogy.” Good writing is good teaching.
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 4
Writing Your Article
The Shape of an Article
An article is written in the shape of an hourglass. It begins with broad general statements, progressively narrows down to
the specifics of your study, and then broadens out again to more general considerations. Thus:
The introduction begins broadly:
“Individuals differ radically from one another in the degree
to which they are willing and able to express their emo-
tions.”
It becomes more specific:
“Indeed, the popular view is that such emotional expressive-
ness is a central difference between men and women.... But
the research evidence is mixed...”
And more so:
“There is even some evidence that men may actually...”
Until you are ready to introduce your own study in concep-
tual terms:
“In this study, we recorded the emotional reactions of both
men and women to filmed...”
The method and results sections are the most specific, the
“neck” of the hourglass:
(Method) One hundred male and 100 female undergraduates
were shown one of two movies...”
“(Results) Table 1 shows that men in the father-watching
condition cried significantly more...”
The discussion section begins with the implications of your
study:
“These results imply that sex differences in emotional ex-
pressiveness are moderated by two kinds of variables...”
It becomes broader:
“Not since Charles Darwin’s first observations has psychol-
ogy contributed as much new...”
And more so:
“If emotions can incarcerate us by hiding our complexity, at
least their expression can liberate us by displaying our
authenticity.”
This closing statement might be a bit grandiose for
some journals—I’m not even sure what it means—but if
your study is carefully executed and conservatively inter-
preted, most editors will permit you to indulge yourself a
bit at the two broad ends of the hourglass. Being dull only
appears to be a prerequisite for publishing in the profes-
sional journals.
The Introduction
The Opening Statements. The first task of the article
is to introduce the background and nature of the problem
being investigated. Here are four rules of thumb for your
opening statements:
1. Write in English prose, not psychological jargon.
2. Do not plunge unprepared readers into the middle
of your problem or theory. Take the time and space nec-
essary to lead them up to the formal or theoretical state-
ment of the problem step by step.
3. Use examples to illustrate theoretical points or to
introduce unfamiliar conceptual or technical terms. The
more abstract the material, the more important such ex-
amples become.
4. Whenever possible, try to open with a statement
about people (or animals), not psychologists or their re-
search (This rule is almost always violated. Don’t use
journals as a model here.)
Examples of Opening Statements:
Wrong: Several years ago, Ekman (1972), Izard
(1977), Tomkins (1980), and Zajonc (1980) pointed to
psychology’s neglect of the affects and their expression.
[Okay for somewhere in the introduction, but not the
opening statement.]
Right: Individuals differ radically from one another in
the degree to which they are willing and able to express
their emotions.
Wrong: Research in the forced-compliance paradigm
has focused on the effects of predecisional alternatives
and incentive magnitude.
Wrong: Festinger’s theory of cognitive dissonance
received a great deal of attention during the latter part of
the twentieth century.
Right: The individual who holds two beliefs that are
inconsistent with one another may feel uncomfortable.
For example, the person who knows that he or she enjoys
smoking but believes it to be unhealthy may experience
discomfort arising from the inconsistency or disharmony
between these two thoughts or cognitions. This feeling of
discomfort was called cognitive dissonance by social psy-
chologist Leon Festinger (1957), who suggested that indi-
viduals will be motivated to remove this dissonance in
whatever way they can.
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 5
Note how this last example leads the reader from fa-
miliar terms (beliefs, inconsistency, discomfort, thoughts)
through transition terms (disharmony, cognitions) to the
unfamiliar technical term cognitive dissonance, thereby
providing an explicit, if nontechnical, definition of it. The
following example illustrates how one might define a
technical term (ego control) and identify its conceptual
status (a personality variable) more implicitly:
The need to delay gratification, control impulses, and
modulate emotional expression is the earliest and most
ubiquitous demand that society places upon the devel-
oping child. And because success at so many of life’s
tasks depends critically upon the individual’s mastery
of such ego control, evidence for life-course continui-
ties in this central personality domain should be read-
ily obtained.
And finally, here is an example in which the techni-
cal terms are defined only by the context. Note, however,
that the technical abbreviation, MAO, is still identified
explicitly when it is first introduced.
In the continuing search for the biological correlates of
psychiatric disorder, blood platelets are now a prime
target of investigation. In particular, reduced mono-
amine oxidase (MAO) activity in the platelets is
sometimes correlated with paranoid symptomatology,
auditory hallucinations or delusions in chronic schizo-
phrenia, and a tendency towards psychopathology in
non-clinical samples of men. Unfortunately, these ob-
servations have not always replicated, casting doubt
on the hypothesis that MAO activity is, in fact, a bio-
logical marker in psychiatric disorder. Even the gen-
eral utility of the platelet model as a key to central
nervous system abnormalities in schizophrenia re-
mains controversial. The present study attempts to
clarify the relation of MAO activity to symptomatol-
ogy in chronic schizophrenia.
This kind of writing would not appear in Newsweek,
and yet it is still comprehensible to an intelligent layper-
son who may know nothing about blood platelets, MAO
activity, or biological markers. The structure of the writ-
ing itself adequately defines the relationships among these
things and provides enough context to make the basic idea
of the study and its rationale clear. At the same time, this
introduction is not condescending nor will it bore the
technically sophisti-cated reader. The pedagogy that
makes this introduction accessible to the nonspecialist
will not only be transparent to the specialist, but will en-
hance the clarity of the article for both readers.
Examples of Examples. When developing complex
conceptual arguments or introducing technical materials,
it is important not only to provide your readers with illus-
trative examples, but to select the examples with care. In
particular, you should try to compose one or two exam-
ples that anticipate your actual findings and then use them
recurrently to make several interrelated conceptual points.
For example, in one of my own studies of trait consis-
tency, some participants were consistently friendly but not
consistently conscientious (Bem & Allen, 1974). Ac-
cordingly, we used examples of friendliness and consci-
entiousness throughout the introduction to clarify and
illustrate our theoretical points about the subtleties of trait
consistency. This pedagogical technique strengthens the
thematic coherence of an article and silently prepares the
reader for understanding the results. It also shortens the
article by removing the need to explain the theory once in
the introduction with hypothetical examples and then
again in the context of the actual results.
This article you are now reading itself provides ex-
amples of recurring examples. Although you do not know
it yet, the major example will be the fictitious study of sex
differences in emotional expression introduced earlier to
illustrate the hourglass shape of an article. I deliberately
constructed the study and provided a sufficient overview
of it at the beginning so that I could draw upon it
throughout the article. Watch for its elaboration as we
proceed. I chose dissonance theory as a second example
because most psychologists are already familiar with it; I
can draw upon this shared resource without having to
expend a lot of space explaining it. But just in case you
are not familiar with it, I introduced it first in the context
of “examples of opening statements” where I could bring
you in from the beginning—just as you should do with
your own readers. And finally, the Bem-Allen article on
trait consistency, mentioned in the previous paragraph,
has some special attributes that will earn it additional
cameo appearances as we continue.
The Literature Review. After making the opening
statements, summarize the current state of knowledge in
the area of investigation. What previous research has been
done on the problem? What are the pertinent theories of
the phenomenon? Although you will have familiarized
yourself with the literature before you designed your own
study, you may need to look up additional references if
your results raise a new aspect of the problem or lead you
to recast the study in a different framework. For example,
if you discover an unanticipated sex difference in your
data, you will want to determine if others have reported a
similar sex difference or findings that might explain it. If
you consider this finding important, discuss sex differ-
ences and the pertinent literature in the introduction. If
you consider it to be only a peripheral finding, then post-
pone a discussion of sex differences until the discussion
section.
The Publication Manual gives the following guide-
lines for the literature review:
Discuss the literature but do not include an exhaustive
historical review. Assume that the reader is knowl-
edgeable about the field for which you are writing and
does not require a complete digest. . . . [C]ite and ref-
erence only works pertinent to the specific issue and
not works of only tangential or general significance. If
you summarize earlier works, avoid nonessential de-
tails; instead, emphasize pertinent findings, relevant
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 6
methodological issues, and major conclusions. Refer
the reader to general surveys or reviews of the topic if
they are available. (APA, 2001, p. 16)
The Publication Manual also urges authors not to let
the goal of brevity mislead them into writing a statement
intelligible only to the specialist. One technique for de-
scribing even an entire study succinctly without sacrific-
ing clarity is to describe one variation of the procedure in
chronological sequence, letting it convey the overview of
the study at the same time. (You can use the same tech-
nique in your own method section.) Here, for example, is
a possible summary of a complicated but classic experi-
ment on cognitive dissonance theory (Festinger & Carls-
mith, 1959):
Sixty male undergraduates were randomly assigned to
one of three conditions. In the $1 condition, the par-
ticipant was first required to perform long repetitive
laboratory tasks in an individual experimental session.
He was then hired by the experimenter as an “assis-
tant” and paid $1 to tell a waiting fellow student (a
confederate) that the tasks were fun and interesting. In
the $20 condition, each participant was hired for $20
to do the same thing. In the control condition, partici-
pants simply engaged in the tasks. After the experi-
ment, each participant indicated on a questionnaire
how much he had enjoyed the tasks. The results
showed that $1 participants rated the tasks as signifi-
cantly more enjoyable than did the $20 participants,
who, in turn, did not differ from the control partici-
pants.
This kind of condensed writing looks easy. It is not,
and you will have to rewrite such summaries repeatedly
before they are both clear and succinct. The preceding
paragraph was my eighth draft.
Citations. The standard journal format permits you to
cite authors in the text either by enclosing their last names
and the year of publication in parentheses, as in A below,
or by using their names in the sentence itself, as in B.
A. “MAO activity in some individuals with schizo-
phrenia is actually higher than normal (Tse & Tung,
1949).”
B. Tse and Tung (1949) report that MAO activity in
some individuals with schizophrenia is actually higher
than normal.”
In general, you should use form A, consigning your
colleagues to parentheses. Your narrative line should be
about MAO activity in individuals with schizophrenia, not
about Tse and Tung. Occasionally, however, you might
want the focus specifically on the authors or researchers:
“Theophrastus (280 B.C.) implies that persons are con-
sistent across situations, but Montaigne (1580) insists that
they are not. Only Mischel (1968), Peterson (1968), and
Vernon (1964), however, have actually surveyed the evi-
dence in detail.” The point here is that you have a deliber-
ate choice to make. Don’t just intermix the two formats
randomly, paying no attention to the narrative structure.
Criticizing Previous Work. If you take a dim view of
previous research or earlier articles in the domain you
reviewed, feel free to criticize and complain as strongly as
you feel is commensurate with the incompetence you
have uncovered. But criticize the work, not the investiga-
tors or authors. Ad hominem attacks offend editors and
reviewers; moreover, the person you attack is likely to be
asked to serve as one of the reviewers of your article. As a
consequence, your opportunity to address—let alone, of-
fend—readers will be nipped in the bud. I could launch
into a sermonette on communitarian values in science, but
I shall assume that this pragmatic warning is sufficient.
Ending the Introduction. End the introduction with a
brief overview of your own study. This provides a smooth
transition into the method section, which follows immedi-
ately:
Because this sex difference remains elusive, it seemed
desirable to test Zanna’s parental-role theory of emo-
tional expression in a more realistic setting. Accord-
ingly, in the study to be presented here, we exposed
men and women to filmed scenes designed to evoke
either negative or positive emotions and assessed their
emotional reactions when they thought they were be-
ing observed by one or both of their parents. We also
sought to examine the relation of emotional expression
to self-esteem.
The Method Section
The Publication Manual spells out in detail what
needs to be included in the method section of an article.
Here are some additional stylistic suggestions.
If you conducted a fairly complex experiment in
which there was a sequence of procedures or events, it is
helpful to lead the reader through the sequence as if he or
she were a participant. First give the usual overview of
the study, including the description of participants, set-
ting, and variables assessed, but then describe the experi-
ment from the participant’s vantage point. Provide sum-
maries or excerpts of what was actually said to the par-
ticipant, including any rationale or “cover story” that was
given. Describe the relevant aspects of the room. Show
sample items from questionnaires, labels on attitude
scales, copies of stimulus materials or pictures of appara-
tus. If you administered a standard personality test or at-
titude scale, describe its general properties unless it is
very familiar (e.g., the MMPI or the F scale). For exam-
ple: “Participants then filled out the Marlowe-Crowne
Social Desirability Scale, a true-false inventory that
measures the degree to which persons describe them-
selves in socially desirable terms (e.g., ‘I have never
lied’).”
The purpose of all this is to give your readers a feel
for what it was like to be a participant. (This is true even
if you used non-human participants. Thus it is more im-
portant to describe the schedule of reinforcement and the
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 7
inner dimensions of the Skinner Box—what the animal
actually experienced—then its outer dimensions and the
voltage of the power supply.) Such information often
bears importantly on the interpretation of the behavior
observed, and readers should be in a position to arrive at
their own judgments about your conclusions.
Name all groups, variables, and operations with eas-
ily recognized and remembered labels. Do not use abbre-
viations (the AMT5% group) or empty labels (Treatment
3). Instead, tell us about the success group and the failure
group, the father-watching condition and the mother-
watching condition, the teacher sample and the student
sample, and so forth. It is also better to label groups or
treatments in operational rather than theoretical terms. It
is difficult to remember that it was the high dissonance
group that was offered the small incentive and the low
dissonance group that was offered the large incentive. So
tell us instead about the $1 group and the $20 group. You
can remind us of the theoretical interpretation of these
variables later when you discuss the results.
The method and results sections share the responsi-
bility for presenting certain kinds of data that support the
reliability and validity of your substantive findings, and
you must judge where this information fits most smoothly
into the narrative and when the reader can most easily
assimilate it. For example, if you constructed a new per-
sonality scale, you need to tell us about its internal homo-
geneity and other psychometric properties. If you em-
ployed observers, tell us about interjudge agreement. If
you mailed survey questionnaires, give us the return rate
and discuss the possibility that non-respondents differed
from respondents. If you discarded certain participants,
tell us why and how many and discuss the possibility that
this limits or qualifies the conclusions you can draw. In
particular, assure us that they were not all concentrated in
the same experimental condition. (Participants discarded
during data analysis should be discussed in the results
section.)
Discuss participant dropout problems and other diffi-
culties encountered in executing the study only if they
might affect the validity or the interpretation of your re-
sults. Otherwise spare us your tales of woe. Do tell us that
some participants fled your high-stress treatment before
you could assess their physiological response, but do not
tell us that your dog ate your pigeon and you had to redo
the experiment or that you couldn’t run participants Tues-
day night because the custodian inadvertently locked the
building.
Manipulations and procedures that yielded no useful
information should be mentioned if they were adminis-
tered before you collected your main data; their presence
could have affected your findings. Usually it will be suffi-
cient to say that they yielded no information and will not
be discussed further. You probably do not need to men-
tion them at all if they were administered after you col-
lected your main data unless you think that other investi-
gators might try to pursue the same fruitless path. Some-
times, however, a “null” result is surprising or of interest
in its own right. In this case, it should be treated as a
regular datum in your results section.
After presenting the methods you used in your study,
discuss any ethical issues they might raise. If the research
design required you to keep participants uninformed or
even misinformed about the procedures, how did you tell
them about this afterwards? How did you obtain their
prior consent? Were they free to withdraw at any time?
Were they subjected to any embarrassment or discomfort?
What steps were followed to protect their anonymity?
Were you observing people who were unaware of that
fact?
If your study raises any of these issues, you should be
prepared to justify your procedures. Moreover, you need
to assure us that your participants were treated with dig-
nity and that they left your study with their self-esteem
intact and their respect for you and psychology enhanced
rather than diminished. If you used non-human partici-
pants—especially dogs, cats, or primates—then you need
to address analogous questions about their care and treat-
ment.
End the method section with a brief summary of the
procedure and its overall purpose. Your grandmother
should be able to skim the method section without reading
it; the final paragraph should bring her back “on line.”
The Results Section
In short articles or reports of single empirical studies,
the results and discussion are often combined. But if you
need to integrate several different kinds of results or dis-
cuss several general matters, then prepare a separate dis-
cussion section. There is, however, no such thing as a
pure results section without an accompanying narrative.
You cannot just throw numbers at readers and expect
them to retain them in memory until they reach the dis-
cussion. In other words, write the results section in Eng-
lish prose.
Setting the Stage. Before you can present your re-
sults, there are two preliminary matters that need to be
handled. First, you should present evidence that your
study successfully set up the conditions for testing your
hypotheses or answering your questions. If your study
required you to produce one group of participants in a
happy mood and another in a depressed mood, show us in
this section that mood ratings made by the two groups
were significantly different. If you divided your partici-
pants into groups, assure us that these groups did not dif-
fer on some unintended variable that might bear upon the
interpretation of your results (e.g., social class, intelli-
gence). If your study required you to misinform partici-
pants about the procedures, how do you know that they
were not suspicious, that participants who participated
earlier had not informed participants who participated
later, and that your “cover story” produced the state of
belief required for the test of your hypotheses?
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 8
Here is also where you can put some of the data dis-
cussed in “The Method Section”: Reliabilities of testing
instruments, judges, and observers; return rates on mail
surveys; and participant dropout problems.
Not all of these matters need to be discussed at the
beginning of the results section. In addition to data you
think fit better in the method section, some of these other
matters might better be postponed until the discussion
section when you are considering alternative explanations
of your results (e.g., the possibility that some participants
became suspicious). Again, the decision of what to in-
clude is very much a matter of judgment. It is an impor-
tant step, but do not overdo it. Get it out of the way as
quickly as possible and get on with your story.
The second preliminary matter to deal with is the
method of data analysis. First, describe any overall proce-
dures you used to convert your raw observations into
analyzable data. How were responses to your mail survey
coded for analysis? How were observers’ ratings com-
bined? Were all measures first converted to standard
scores? Some of these may also fit better into the method
section and need not be repeated. Similarly, data-
combining procedures that are highly specific can be
postponed. If you combined three measures of anxiety
into a single composite score for analysis, tell us about
that later when you are about to present the anxiety data.
Next, tell us about the statistical analysis itself. If this
is standard, describe it briefly (e.g., “All data were ana-
lyzed by two-way analyses of variance with sex of par-
ticipant and mood induction as the independent vari-
ables”). If the analysis is unconventional or makes certain
statistical assumptions your data might not satisfy, how-
ever, discuss the rationale for it, perhaps citing a reference
for readers who wish to check into it further. If your
method of analysis is new or likely to be unfamiliar to
readers of the journal, you might need to provide a full
explanation of it. Sometimes the quantitative treatment of
data is a major part of an article’s contribution. Variations
of multidimensional scaling, causal modeling, and cir-
cumplex representations of personality data, for example,
have been more important in some articles than the data to
which they were applied. In these cases, the method of
analysis and its rationale have the same epistemological
status as a theory and should be presented in the introduc-
tion to the article.
And finally, if the results section is complicated or
divided into several parts, you may wish to provide an
overview of the section: “The results are presented in
three parts. The first section presents the behavioral re-
sults for the men, followed by the parallel results for the
women. The final section presents the attitudinal and
physiological data for both sexes combined.” But as I
shall argue, this kind of “metacommentary” should be
used sparingly. In most cases, the prose itself should
make it unnecessary.
Presenting the Findings. The general rule in report-
ing your findings is to give the forest first and then the
trees. This is true of the results section as a whole: Begin
with the central findings, and then move to more periph-
eral ones. It is also true within subsections: State the basic
finding first, and then elaborate or qualify it as necessary.
Similarly, discuss an overall measure of aggression or
whatever first, and then move to its individual compo-
nents. Beginning with one of your most central results,
proceed as follows:
1. Remind us of the conceptual hypothesis or the
question you are asking: “It will be recalled that the men
are expected to be more emotionally expressive than the
women.” Or, “We ask, first, whether the men or the
women are more emotionally expressive?” Note that this
is a conceptual statement of the hypothesis or question.
2. Remind us of the operations performed and be-
haviors measured: “In particular, the men should produce
more tears during the showing of the film than the
women.” Or, “Do the men produce more tears during the
showing of the film than the women?” Note that this is an
operational statement of the hypothesis or question.
3. Tell us the answer immediately and in English:
“The answer is yes.” Or, “As Table 1 reveals, men do, in
fact, cry more profusely than the women.”
4. Now, and only now, speak to us in numbers. (Your
grandmother can now skip to the next result in case she
has forgotten her statistics or her reading glasses.): “Thus
the men in all four conditions produced an average of 1.4
cc more tears than the women, F (1,112) = 5.79, p <
.025.”
5. Now you may elaborate or qualify the overall con-
clusion if necessary: “Only in the father-watching condi-
tion did the men fail to produce more tears than the
women, but a specific test of this effect failed to reach
significance, t =1.58, p < .12.”
6. End each section of the results with a summary of
where things stand: “Thus, except for the father-watching
condition, which will be discussed below, the hypothesis
that men cry more than women in response to visually-
depicted grief appears to receive strong support.”
7. Lead into the next section of the results with a
smooth transition sentence: “Men may thus be more ex-
pressive than women in the domain of negative emotion,
but are they more expressive in the domain of positive
emotion? Table 2 shows they are not...” (Again, the
“bottom line” is given immediately.) As the results sec-
tion proceeds, continue to summarize and “update” the
reader’s store of information frequently. The reader
should not have to keep looking back to retrieve the major
points of your plot line.
By structuring the results section in this way, by
moving from forest to trees, by announcing each result
clearly in prose before wading into numbers and statistics,
and by summarizing frequently, you permit a reader to
decide just how much detail he or she wants to pursue at
each juncture and to skip ahead to the next main point
whenever that seems desirable.
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 9
Figures and Tables. Unless a set of findings can be
stated in one or two numbers, results that are sufficiently
important to be stressed should be accompanied by a fig-
ure or table summarizing the relevant data. The basic rule
of presentation is that a reader be able to grasp your major
findings either by reading the text or by looking at the
figures and tables. Thus, figures and tables must be titled
and labeled clearly and completely, even if that means
constructing a lengthy title or heading (“Mean number of
tears produced by two affective films as a function of
affect valence, participant sex, parental observation, and
self-esteem”). Within the text itself, lead the reader by the
hand through a table to point out the results of interest:
“As shown in the first column of Table 2, men produce
more tears (2.33 cc) than women (1.89 cc).... Of particular
interest is the number of tears produced when both father
and mother watch (rows 3 and 4)...” Do not just wave in
the general direction of the table and expect the reader to
ferret out the information. For detailed information on
figures and tables, see the Publication Manual (APA,
2001).
On Statistics. As you know, every comparison be-
tween groups or relationship between variables should be
accompanied by its level of statistical significance. Oth-
erwise, readers have no way of knowing whether the
findings could have emerged by chance. But despite the
importance of inferential statistics, they are not the heart
of your narrative and should be subordinated to the de-
scriptive results. Whenever possible, state a result first
and then give its statistical significance, but in no case
should you ever give the statistical test alone without in-
terpreting it substantively. Do not tell us that the three-
way interaction with sex, parent condition, and self-
esteem was significant at the .05 level unless you tell us
immediately and in English that men are less expressive
than women in the negative conditions if father
watches—but only for men with low self-esteem.
If your experiment used an analysis of variance de-
sign, your data analysis will automatically display the
effects of several independent variables on a single de-
pendent variable. If this organization is consonant with a
smooth presentation of your results, lucky you. Go with it.
But do not be a prisoner of ANOVA! If the narrative
flows more smoothly by discussing the effects of a single
independent variable on several conceptually related de-
pendent variables, tear your ANOVA results apart and
reorganize them. Statistical designs are all right in their
place, but you—and your prose—are master; they are
slave.
Just as your method section should give readers a feel
for the procedures used, so too, the results section should
give them a feel for the behavior observed. Select de-
scriptive indexes or statistics that convey the behavior of
your participants as vividly as possible. Tell us the per-
cent of children in your study who hit the Bobo doll or the
mean number of times they did so. Remind us that a score
of 3.41 on your 5-point rating scale of aggression lies
between “slightly aggressive” and “moderately aggres-
sive.”
Do this even if the statistical analyses must be per-
formed on some more indirect datum (e.g., the arcsin
transform of the number of Bobo hits or the sum of three
standardized aggression scores.) Display these indirect
indexes, too, if you wish, but give the readers’ intuitions
first priority. For example, in our study of trait consis-
tency, we analyzed a standard-score index of individual
consistency, but we discussed the results in terms of the
more familiar correlation coefficient—on which no le-
gitimate statistical analysis could be performed (Bem &
Allen, 1974).
After you have presented your quantitative results, it
is often useful to become more informal and briefly to
describe the behavior of particular individuals in your
study. Again, the point is not to prove something, but to
add richness to your findings, to share with readers the
feel of the behavior: “Indeed, two of the men used an en-
tire box of Kleenex during the showing of the heart op-
eration but would not pet the baby kitten owned by the
secretary.”
The Discussion Section
As noted earlier, the discussion section can either be
combined with the results section or appear separately. In
either case, it forms a cohesive narrative with the intro-
duction, and you should expect to move materials back
and forth between these two sections as you rewrite and
reshape the report. Topics that are central to your story
will appear in the introduction and probably again in the
discussion. More peripheral topics may not be brought up
at all until after the presentation of the results. The discus-
sion is also the bottom of the hourglass-shaped format and
thus proceeds from specific matters about your study to
more general concerns (about methodological strategies,
for example) to the broadest generalizations you wish to
make. The sequence of topics is often the mirror image of
the sequence in the introduction.
Begin the discussion by telling us what you have
learned from the study. Open with a clear statement on
the support or nonsupport of the hypotheses or the an-
swers to the questions you first raised in the introduction.
But do not simply reformulate and repeat points already
summarized in the results section. Each new statement
should contribute something new to the reader’s under-
standing of the problem. What inferences can be drawn
from the findings? These inferences may be at a level
quite close to the data or may involve considerable ab-
straction, perhaps to the level of a larger theory regarding,
say, emotion or sex differences. What are the theoretical,
practical, or even the political implications of the results?
It is also appropriate at this point to compare your re-
sults with those reported by other investigators and to
discuss possible shortcomings of your study, conditions
that might limit the extent of legitimate generalization or
otherwise qualify your inferences. Remind readers of the
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 10
characteristics of your participant sample, the possibility
that it might differ from other populations to which you
might want to generalize; of specific characteristics of
your methods that might have influenced the outcome; or
of any other factors that might have operated to produce
atypical results.
But do not dwell compulsively on every flaw! In par-
ticular, be willing to accept negative or unexpected results
without a tortured attempt to explain them away. Do not
make up long, involved, pretzel-shaped theories to ac-
count for every hiccup in the data. There is a -.73 correla-
tion between the clarity of an investigator’s results and
the length of his or her discussion section. Do not con-
tribute to this shameful statistic.
Ah, but suppose that, on the contrary, your results
have led you to a grand new theory that injects startling
clarity into your data and revolutionizes your view of the
problem area. Doesn’t this justify a long discussion sec-
tion? No. In this case, you should write the article so that
you begin with your new theory. As noted above, your
task is to provide the most informative and compelling
framework for your study from the opening sentence. If
your new theory does that, don’t wait until the discussion
section to spring it on us. A journal article is not a chro-
nology of your thought processes.
The discussion section also includes a consideration
of questions that remain unanswered or that have been
raised by the study itself, along with suggestions for the
kinds of research that would help to answer them. In fact,
suggesting additional research is probably the most com-
mon way of ending a research report.
Common, but dull. The hourglass shape of an article
implies that your final words should be broad general
statements of near-cosmic significance, not precious de-
tails of interest only to psychologists. Thus the statement,
“Further research will be needed before it is clear whether
the androgyny scale should be scored as a single continu-
ous dimension or partitioned into a 4-way typology,”
might well be appropriate somewhere in a discussion sec-
tion, but, please, not your final farewell. In my opinion,
only Montaigne was clever enough to end an article with
a statement about further research: “Because [the study of
motivation] is a high and hazardous undertaking, I wish
fewer people would meddle with it” (1580/1943).
You should probably settle for more modest injunc-
tions: “If gender schema theory has a political message, it
is.. .that... human behaviors and personality attributes
should no longer be linked with gender, and society
should stop projecting gender into situations irrelevant to
genitalia. The feminist prescription, then, is not that the
individual be androgynous, but that the society be gender-
aschematic” (S. Bem, 1985).
But in any case, end with a bang, not a whimper.
The Title and Abstract
The title and abstract of your article permit potential
readers to get a quick overview of your study and to de-
cide if they wish to read the article itself. Titles and ab-
stracts are also indexed and compiled in reference works
and computerized databases. For this reason they should
accurately reflect the content of the article and include
key words that will ensure their retrieval from a database.
You should compose the title and abstract after you have
completed the article and have a firm view of its structure
and content.
The recommended length for a title is 10 to 12 words.
It should be fully explanatory when standing alone and
identify the theoretical issues or the variables under in-
vestigation. Because you will not be able to mention all
the features of your study in the title (or even in the ab-
stract), you must decide which are most important. Once
again, the data should guide you. For example, the most
instructive findings from our fictitious study on emotional
expression should determine which of the following is the
most appropriate title: “Laughing versus Crying: Sex Dif-
ferences in the Public Display of Positive and Negative
Emotions”; “Effects of Being Observed by Parents on the
Emotional Responses of Men and Women to Visual
Stimuli”; “Emotional Responses to Visual Stimuli as a
Function of Sex and Self-Esteem”; “Sex Differences in
the Public Display of Emotion as a Function of the Ob-
serving Audience”; “Public versus Private Displays of
Emotion in Men and Women.”
The abstract of an empirical article should not exceed
120 words. It should contain the problem under investi-
gation (in one sentence if possible); the participants,
specifying pertinent characteristics, such as number, type,
age, sex, and species; the experimental method, including
the apparatus, data-gathering procedures, and complete
test names; the findings, including statistical significance
levels; and the conclusion and the implications or appli-
cations.
Clearly the abstract must be compact, and this re-
quirement leads many inexperienced writers to make it
unintelligible. Remove unnecessary words and eliminate
less important details of method and results. But then let it
breathe. In particular, allow yourself the space to make
the problem under investigation clear to a casually
browsing reader. Often you can plagiarize and abbreviate
key statements from the article itself. Here is an example:
When are men more emotionally expressive than
women? One hundred male and 100 female under-
graduates were individually shown a sad or a happy
film, while being observed by one or both of their par-
ents. Judges blind to condition rated participants’ fa-
cial expressions, and a Lachrymeter measured their
tear volume. Men cried more during the sad movie but
laughed less during the happy movie than did the
women (interaction, p < .02). However, men in the
father-watching condition with low self-esteem (Dar-
ley Self-Concept Scale) cried less than all other par-
ticipants (p < .05). It is suggested that sex differences
in emotional expression are moderated by the valence
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 11
of the emotion and—for men—by self-esteem and
conditions of being observed.
If the conceptual contribution of your article is more
important than the supporting study, this can be reflected
in the abstract by omitting experimental details and giving
more space to the theoretical material. Here is the title and
abstract from our Psychological Review article on trait
consistency (revised to conform to the new 100 word
limit on abstracts of reviews and theoretical articles):
On Predicting Some of the People Some of the Time:
The Search for Cross-Situational Consistencies in
Behavior
The recurring controversy over the existence of cross-
situational consistencies in behavior is sustained by
the discrepancy between our intuitions, which affirm
their existence, and the research literature, which does
not. It is argued that the nomothetic assumptions of the
traditional research paradigm are incorrect and that
higher validity coefficients would be obtained if the
idiographic assumptions used by our intuitions were
adopted. A study is reported which shows it is possible
to predict who will be cross-situationally consistent
and who will not. Personality assessment must not
only attend to situations—as has been recently
urged—but to persons as well (Bem & Allen, 1974).
Rewriting and Polishing Your Article
For many authors revising an article is unmitigated
agony. Even proofreading is painful. And so they don’t.
So relieved to get a draft done, they send it off to the
journal thinking that they can clean up the writing after it
has been accepted. Alas, that day rarely comes. Some may
find solace in the belief that the manuscript probably
would have been rejected even if it had been extensively
revised and polished; after all, most of psychology jour-
nals accept only 15 to 20% of all manuscripts submitted.
But from my experience as an editor, I believe that the
difference between the manuscripts accepted and the top
15 to 20% of those rejected is frequently the difference
between good and less good writing. Moral: Do not ex-
pect journal reviewers to discern your brilliance through
the smog of polluted writing. Revise your manuscript.
Polish it. Proofread it. Then submit it.
Rewriting is difficult for several reasons. First, it is
difficult to edit your own writing. You will not notice
ambiguities and explanatory gaps because you know what
you meant to say and you understand the omitted steps.
One strategy for overcoming this difficulty is to lay your
manuscript aside for awhile and then return to it later
when it has become less familiar. Sometimes it helps to
read it aloud. But there is no substitute for practicing the
art of taking the role of the nonspecialist reader, for
learning to role-play grandma. As you read, ask yourself,
“Have I been told yet what this concept means?” Has the
logic of this step been demonstrated?” “Would I know
what the independent variable is at this point?” This is
precisely the skill of the good lecturer in Psychology 101,
the ability to anticipate the audience’s level of under-
standing at each point in the presentation. Good writing is
good teaching.
But because this is not easy, you should probably
give a fairly polished copy of the manuscript to a friend or
colleague for a critical review. (If you get a critique from
two colleagues you will have simulated a trial run of a
journal’s review process.) The best readers are those who
have themselves published in the psychological journals,
but who are unfamiliar with the subject of your article. (A
student from Psychology 101 would probably be too in-
timidated to give usefully critical feedback; grandma will
be too kind.)
If your colleagues find something unclear, do not ar-
gue with them. They are right: By definition, the writing
is unclear. Their suggestions for correcting the unclarities
may be wrong, even dumb. But as unclarity detectors,
readers are never wrong. Also resist the temptation simply
to clarify their confusion verbally. Your colleagues do not
want to offend you or appear stupid, and so they will sim-
ply mumble “oh yes, of course, of course” and apologize
for not having read carefully enough. As a consequence,
you will be pacified, and your next readers, the journal
reviewers, will stumble over the same problem. They will
not apologize; they will reject.
Rewriting is difficult for a second reason: It requires
a high degree of compulsiveness and attention to detail.
The probability of writing a sentence perfectly the first
time is vanishingly small, and good writers rewrite nearly
every sentence of an article in the course of polishing
successive drafts. But even good writers differ from one
another in their approach to the first draft. Some spend a
long time carefully choosing each word and reshaping
each sentence and paragraph as they go. Others pound out
a rough draft quickly and then go back for extensive revi-
sion. Although I personally prefer the former method, I
think it wastes time. For journal articles in particular, I
think most authors should get the first draft done as
quickly as possible without agonizing over stylistic nice-
ties. But once it is done, compulsiveness and attention to
detail become the required virtues.
Finally, rewriting is difficult because it usually means
restructuring. Sometimes it is necessary to discard whole
sections of an article, add new ones, go back and do more
data analysis, and then totally reorganize the article just to
iron out a bump in the logic of the argument. Do not get
so attached to your first draft that you are unwilling to
tear it apart and rebuild it. (This is why the technique of
crafting each sentence of a first draft wastes time. That
beautiful turn of phrase that took me 40 minutes to shape
gets discarded when the article gets restructured. Worse, I
get so attached to the phrase that I resist restructuring
until I can find a new home for it.) A badly constructed
building cannot be salvaged by brightening up the wall-
paper. A badly constructed article cannot be salvaged by
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 12
changing words, inverting sentences, and shuffling para-
graphs.
Which brings me to the word processor. Its very vir-
tuosity at making these cosmetic changes may tempt you
to tinker endlessly, encouraging you in the illusion that
you are restructuring right there in front of the monitor.
Do not be fooled. You are not. A word processor—even
in conjunction with a fancy “outline mode”—is not an
adequate restructuring tool. Moreover, it can produce
flawless, physically beautiful drafts of wretched writing,
encouraging you in the illusion that they are finished
manuscripts ready to be submitted. Do not be fooled.
They are not. If you are blessed with an excellent memory
(or a very large monitor) and are confident that you can
get away with a purely electronic process of restructuring,
fine, do it. But do not be ashamed to print out a complete
draft of your manuscript, spread it out on table or floor,
take pencil, scissors, and scotch tape in hand, and then, all
by your low-tech self, have at it.
Some Matters of Style
Omit Needless Words
Virtually all experienced writers agree that any writ-
ten expression that deserves to be called vigorous writing,
whether it is a short story, an article for a professional
journal, or a complete book, is characterized by the attrib-
ute of being succinct, concise, and to the point. A sen-
tence—no matter where in the writing it occurs—should
contain no unnecessary or superfluous words, words that
stand in the way of the writer’s direct expression of his or
her meaning and purpose. In a very similar fashion, a
paragraph—the basic unit of organization in English
prose—should contain no unnecessary or superfluous
sentences, sentences that introduce peripheral content into
the writing or stray from its basic narrative line. It is in
this sense that a writer is like an artist executing a draw-
ing, and it is in this sense that a writer is like an engineer
designing a machine. Good writing should be economical
for the same reason that a drawing should have no un-
necessary lines, and good writing should be streamlined
in the same way that a machine is designed to have no
unnecessary parts, parts that contribute little or nothing to
its intended function.
This prescription to be succinct and concise is often
misunderstood and requires judicious application. It cer-
tainly does not imply that the writer must make all of his
or her sentences short and choppy or leave out all adjec-
tives, adverbs, and qualifiers. Nor does it mean that he or
she must avoid or eliminate all detail from the writing
and treat his or her subjects only in the barest skeleton or
outline form. But the requirement does imply that every
word committed to paper should tell something new to the
reader and contribute in a significant and non-redundant
way to the message that the writer is trying to convey.
* * *
You have just read a 303 word essay on brevity. It is
not a terrible first draft, but a good writer or copy editor
would take its message to heart and, by crossing out all
the non-italicized words, cut it by 81%. Savor the result:
Vigorous writing is concise. A sentence should con-
tain no unnecessary words, a paragraph no unneces-
sary sentences, for the same reason that a drawing
should have no unnecessary lines and a machine no
unnecessary parts. This requires not that the writer
make all sentences short or avoid all detail and treat
subjects only in outline, but that every word tell. [59
words]
This essay on brevity was written by Strunk and
White (2000, p. 23) under the heading: “Omit Needless
Words.” Obey their injunction, because it is the most im-
portant piece of advice in this article. Journal articles
should also omit needless concepts, topics, anecdotes,
asides, and footnotes. Clear any underbrush that clutters
your narrative. If a point seems peripheral to your main
theme, remove it. If you cannot bring yourself to do this,
put it in a footnote. Then when you revise your manu-
script, remove the footnote.
Copy editing other people’s writing is good practice
for improving your own. It is also less painful than editing
your own and much easier than actually writing. Any
piece of prose will do. Here was an exercise for my writ-
ing class; it was part of a letter Cornell sent out to poten-
tial graduate applicants. You may wish to try your hand at
it.
Psychology is a wide field of study, and we are not
equally strong in all parts of it. At present, we regard
our major strengths as lying in three broadly defined
domains in which we have many faculty, and a couple
of smaller areas in which also have appreciable re-
sources. The three primary areas are Biopsychology,
Experimental Psychology, and Personality and Social
Psychology; the others are Mathematical/Differential
Psychology and Experimental Psychopathology. The
areas and the relevant faculty are listed below. Please
note that this listing is informal; it does not imply that
the listed faculty members have no other interests or
can readily be fitted into predefined areas. The actual
network of faculty interests and responsibilities is too
subtle to be described in a letter such as this. The list-
ing is just a rough and ready way to tell you what the
Field of Psychology at Cornell is like. [149 words]
Here is a reasonable revision:
Psychology is a wide field, and our major strengths are
Biopsychology, Experimental Psychology, and Per-
sonality and Social Psychology. We also have re-
sources in Mathematical/Differential Psychology and
Experimental Psychopathology. The following list of
faculty within areas provides a rough guide to the
Field of Psychology at Cornell. Faculty interests are
broader than this list implies, however, and do not al-
ways neatly fit the predefined areas. [65 words, a
savings of 56%]
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 13
To maintain the vigor of your prose, try to spend at
least 15 minutes each day omitting needless words. Your
goal should be to reach at least 30% of all words encoun-
tered. (Copy edited versions of this article will be returned
unopened.)
Avoid Metacomments on the Writing
Expository writing fails its mission if it diverts the
reader’s attention to itself and away from the topic; the
process of writing should be transparent to the reader. In
particular, the prose itself should direct the flow of the
narrative without requiring you to play tour guide by
commenting on it. Do not say, “Now that I have discussed
the three theories of emotion, we can turn to the empirical
work on each of them. I will begin with the psychoana-
lytic account of affect...” Instead, move directly from your
discussion of the theories into the literature review with a
simple transition sentence such as, “Each of these three
theories has been tested empirically. Thus, the psycho-
analytic account of affect has received support in studies
that...” Do not say, “Now that we have seen the results for
negative affect, we are in a position to examine men’s and
women’s emotional expression in the realm of positive
affect. The relevant data are presented in Table 2...” In-
stead use a transition sentence that simultaneously sum-
marizes and moves the story along: “Men may thus be
more expressive than women in the domain of negative
emotion, but are they also more expressive in the domain
of positive emotion? Table 2 shows that they are not...”
Any other guideposts needed can be supplied by using
informative headings and by following the advice on
repetition and parallel construction given in the next sec-
tion.
If you feel the need to make metacomments to keep
the reader on the narrative path, then your plot line is
probably already too cluttered, the writing insufficiently
linear. Metacomments will only oppress the prose further.
Instead, copy edit. Omit needless words; don’t add them!
Use Repetition and Parallel Construction
Inexperienced writers often substitute synonyms for
recurring words and vary their sentence structure in the
mistaken belief that this is more creative, stylish, or inter-
esting. Instead of using repetition and parallel construc-
tion, as in “Men may be more expressive than women in
the domain of negative emotion, but they are not more
expressive in the domain of positive emotion,” they at-
tempt to be more creative: “Men may be more expressive
than women in the domain of negative emotion, but it is
not true that they are more willing and able than the oppo-
site sex to display the more cheerful affects.”
Such creativity is hardly more interesting, but it is
certainly more confusing. In scientific communication, it
can be deadly. When an author uses different words to
refer to the same concept in a technical article—where
accuracy is paramount—readers will justifiably wonder if
different meanings are implied. The example above is not
disastrous, and most readers will be unaware that their
understanding flickered momentarily when the prose hit a
bump. But consider the cognitive burden carried by read-
ers who must hack through this “creative” jungle:
The high-dissonance participants were paid a small
sum of money while being given a free choice of
whether or not to participate, whereas the participants
we randomly assigned to the large-incentive treatment
(the low-dissonance condition) were not offered the
opportunity to refuse.
This (fictitious) author should have written:
High dissonance participants were paid a small sum of
money and were not required to participate; low-
dissonance participants were paid a large sum of
money and were required to participate.
The wording and grammatical structure of the two
clauses are held rigidly parallel; only the variables vary.
Repetition and parallel construction are among the most
effective servants of clarity. Don’t be creative, be clear.
Repetition and parallel construction also serve clarity
at a larger level of organization. By providing the reader
with distinctive guideposts to the structure of the prose,
they can diminish or eliminate the need for metacom-
ments. Here, for example, are the opening sentences from
three of the paragraphs in the earlier section on rewriting:
2nd paragraph: “Rewriting is difficult for several rea-
sons. First...”
5th paragraph: “Rewriting is difficult for a second
reason:”
6th paragraph: “Finally, rewriting is difficult because
it...”
If I had substituted synonyms for the recurring words
or varied the grammatical structure of these opening sen-
tences, their guiding function would have been lost, the
reader’s sense of the section’s organization blurred. (I try
so hard to be helpful, and I bet you didn’t even notice.
That, of course, is the point.)
Finally, repetition and parallel construction can serve
style and creativity as well as clarity. They can provide
rhythm and punch: “A sentence should contain no unnec-
essary words, a paragraph no unnecessary sentences for
the same reason that a drawing should have no unneces-
sary lines and a machine no unnecessary parts.” They can
establish metaphor: “A badly constructed building cannot
be salvaged by brightening up the wallpaper. A badly
constructed article cannot be salvaged by changing words,
inverting sentences, and shuffling paragraphs.” They can
add humor: “The word processor encourages you in the
illusion that you are restructuring. Do not be fooled. You
are not. The word processor encourages you in the illu-
sion that your drafts are finished manuscripts. Do not be
fooled. They are not.”
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 14
Jargon
Jargon is the specialized vocabulary of a discipline,
and it serves a number of legitimate functions in scientific
communication. A specialized term may be more general,
more precise, or freer of surplus meaning than any natural
language equivalent (e.g., the term disposition encom-
passes, and hence is more general than, beliefs, attitudes,
moods, and personality attributes; reinforcement is more
precise and freer of surplus meaning than reward). And
the technical vocabulary often makes an important con-
ceptual distinction not apprehended by the layperson’s
vocabulary (e.g., genotype versus phenotype).
But if a jargon term does not satisfy any of these cri-
teria, opt for English. Much psychological jargon has be-
come second-nature to us in the profession and serves
only to muddy our prose for the general reader. (I once
had to interrogate an author at length to learn that a prison
program for “strengthening the executive functions of the
ego” actually taught prisoners how to fill out job applica-
tions.) Unless the jargon term is extremely well known
(e.g., reinforcement), it should be defined—explicitly,
implicitly, or by example—the first time it is introduced.
(See the sample opening statements earlier in this article
for ways to do this.)
Voice and Self-Reference
In the past, scientific writers used the passive voice
almost exclusively and referred to themselves in the third
person: “This experiment was designed by the authors to
test ...” This practice produces lifeless prose and is no
longer the norm. Use the active voice unless style or con-
tent dictates otherwise; and, in general, keep self-
reference to a minimum. Remember that you are not the
topic of your article. You should not refer to yourself as
“the author” or “the investigator.” (You may refer to “the
experimenter” in the method section, however, even if
that happens to be you; the experimenter is part of the
topic under discussion there.) Do not refer to yourself as
“we” unless there really are two or more authors. You
may refer to yourself as “I,” but do so sparingly. It tends
to distract the reader from the topic, and it is better to re-
main in the background. Leave the reader in the back-
ground, too. Do not say, “The reader will find it hard to
believe that ... or “You will be surprised to learn...”
(This article violates the rule because you and your prose
are the topic.) You may, however, refer to the reader indi-
rectly in imperative, “you-understood” sentences: “Con-
sider, first, the results for women.” “Note particularly the
difference between the means in Table 1.”
In some contexts, you can use “we” to refer collec-
tively to yourself and your readers: “We can see in Table
1 that most of the tears...” The Publication Manual, how-
ever, emphasizes that the referent of “we” must be unam-
biguous; for example, copy editors will object to the sen-
tence “In everyday life, of course, we tend to overesti-
mate...” because it is not clear just who is meant by “we.”
They will accept “In everyday life, of course, we humans
tend to overestimate...” or “In everyday life, of course,
human decision makers often make errors; for example,
we tend to overestimate...”
Tense
Use the past or present perfect tense when reporting
the previous research of others (“Bandura reported...” or
“Hardin has reported…”) and past tense when reporting
how you conducted your study (“Observers were posted
behind...”) and specific past behaviors of your participants
(“Two of the men talked...”). Use the present tense for
results currently in front of the reader (“As Table 2
shows, the negative film is more effective ...”) and for
conclusions that are more general than the specific results
(“Positive emotions, then, are more easily expressed
when...”).
Avoid Language Bias
Like most publishers, the APA now has extensive
guidelines for language that refers to individuals or
groups. If your article requires you to discuss any of the
groups mentioned in this section, you should probably
consult the detailed advice in the Publication Manual
(APA, 2001, pp. 61-76)
Research Participants. One distinctive group of peo-
ple who appear in our journal articles are those whom we
study. It is no longer considered appropriate to objectify
them by calling them subjects. Instead use descriptive
terms that either identify them more specifically or that
acknowledge their roles as partners in the research proc-
ess, such as college students, children, individuals, par-
ticipants, interviewees, or respondents. You may still use
the terms subjects, subject variables, and subject sample
when discussing statistics or (at least for now) when refer-
ring to non-human participants.
Sex and Gender. The issue of language bias comes up
most frequently with regard to sex or gender, and the
most awkward problems arise from the use of masculine
nouns and pronouns when the content refers to both sexes.
The generic use of man, he, his, and him to refer to both
sexes is not only misleading in many instances, but re-
search shows that readers think of male persons when
these forms are used (Martyna, 1978). Sometimes the
results are not only sexist, but humorous in their naive
androcentrism: “Man’s vital needs include food, water,
and access to females” (Quoted in Martyna, 1978).
In most contexts, the simplest alternative is the use of
the plural. Instead of writing, “The individual who dis-
plays prejudice in his personal relations...,” write “Indi-
viduals who display prejudice in their personal relations
are ...” Sometimes the pronoun can simply be dropped or
replaced by a sex-neutral article (the, a, or an). Instead of
writing, “The researcher must avoid letting his precon-
ceptions bias his interpretation of results,” you can write,
“The researcher must avoid letting preconceptions bias
the interpretation of results.”
If it is stylistically important to focus on the single
individual, the use of “he or she,” “him or her,” and so
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 15
forth is acceptable but clumsy if used often. Alternating
he and she is both confusing and distracting. Similarly,
alternatives like he/she or s/he are unpronounceable and
grate on the eye. Do not use them.
You may find it instructive to review how I have
dealt with the pronoun problem in this article. In particu-
lar, note the many references to the “reader” or “readers.”
Sometimes the plural worked fine (e.g., “Don’t plunge
readers into the middle of your problem. Take the time to
lead them...”), but in other instances the imagery of the
sentence required the stylistic use of the singular (e.g.,
“Lead the reader by the hand through a table...”). In these
cases, I have tried to minimize using the awkward “he or
she” construction.
Stylistic matters aside, however, you must be accu-
rate in your use of pronouns when you describe your re-
search or that of others. Readers must be explicitly told
the sex of experimenters, observers, and participants.
When referring to males, use male pronouns; when refer-
ring to females use female pronouns. (See, for example,
the earlier description of the Festinger-Carlsmith study,
which used male participants.) Under no circumstances
should you omit or hide sex identity in a misguided at-
tempt to be unbiased.
The problems of gender reference become easier
when we move away from pronouns. Words like man and
mankind are easily replaced by terms like people and hu-
manity. Instead of manning projects, we can staff them.
The federal government has already desexed occupational
titles so that we have letter carriers rather than mailmen;
in private industry we have flight attendants rather than
stewardesses. And in life, children need nurturing or par-
enting, not just mothering. In all these cases, you will find
it easy to discover the appropriate sex-neutral term if you
think of the activity rather than the person doing it.
Next, watch out for plain old stereotyping. The
author who asserts that “research scientists often neglect
their wives” fails to acknowledge that women as well as
men are research scientists. If the author meant specifi-
cally male research scientists, he (she?) should have said
so. Do not talk about ambitious men and aggressive
women or cautious men and timid women if the use of
different adjectives denotes not different behaviors on the
part of men and women but your stereotyped interpreta-
tion of those behaviors. Do not make stereotyped as-
sumptions about marital sex-roles by saying that “The
client’s husband lets her teach part-time” if all you know
is that the client teaches part-time. If the bias is not yours
but someone else’s, let the writing make that clear: “The
client’s husband ‘lets’ her teach part-time.” “The husband
says he ‘lets’ the client teach part-time.” “The client says
her husband lets her teach part-time.” “The client says
sarcastically that her husband ‘lets’ her teach part-time.”
The client and her husband are allowed to say such things.
You are not.
Finally, select examples with care. Beware of your
assumptions about the sex of doctors, homemakers,
nurses, and so forth. Why not: “The athlete who believes
in her ability to succeed...”? Let our writing promote the
view that woman’s vital needs are the same as man’s:
food, water, and access to equality.
Racial and Ethnic Identity. Preferences for names re-
ferring to racial and ethnic groups change over time. For
example, African American and Black are currently ac-
ceptable terms, whereas Negro and Afro-American are
now obsolete. Similarly, Asian and Asian American are
currently acceptable designations, but Oriental is not. As
these examples illustrate, hyphens should not be used in
multiword designations, and terms such as Black and
White are considered proper nouns and should be capital-
ized.
Depending on their historical countries of origin, in-
dividuals may prefer to be called Hispanic, Latino, or
Chicano (Latina and Chicana for women). American In-
dian and Native American are both accepted terms for
referring to indigenous people of North America; but,
technically, only the latter category includes Hawaiians
and Samoans. Native peoples of northern Canada, Alaska,
eastern Siberia, and Greenland may prefer Inuk (plural,
Inuit) to Eskimo. Alaska Natives include many groups in
addition to Eskimos. It is often relevant to be more spe-
cific in describing your participants. For example, it may
be pertinent to know that they were Cuban, not just His-
panic; Chinese, Vietnamese, or Korean, not just Asian.
If you are uncertain about how to describe your re-
search participants, ask them how they prefer to be identi-
fied.
Sexual Orientation. Like terms referring to racial and
ethnic identity, terms referring to sexual orientation also
change over time. For example, the term sexual orienta-
tion itself is now preferred to the older sexual prefer-
ence—which implies a temporary free choice rather than
an enduring disposition. Although terms such as homo-
sexual and homosexuality are still technically correct and
may be used in phrases such as “a homosexual orienta-
tion” or “more Americans now accept homosexuality,”
they should be avoided when referring to individuals or
groups. Instead of referring to homosexuals or bisexuals,
use lesbians, gay men, or bisexual men and women. In
some contexts, the word gay can be used to include both
men and women (e. g., “the gay rights movement”), but
when referring to individuals or groups, retain the dis-
tinction between gay men and lesbians.
Sexual orientation is not the same as sexual behavior.
Not everyone who engages in a sexual act with a person
of the same sex should be considered gay or lesbian, and
hence you should not use the terms homosexual behavior
or heterosexual behavior. Instead, describe specific in-
stances of sexual behavior using terms such as male-male,
female-female, male-female, or same-gender sexual be-
havior.
Note the use of the word gender in the previous sen-
tence. Although there are differences in usage across and
within disciplines, the term sex is usually considered to
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 16
refer to biology; thus male and female are terms referring
to sex. In contrast, gender usually refers to the cultural
interpretation or elaboration of sex; thus masculine and
feminine are terms referring to gender. This is not a hard
and fast rule, however. For example, because the term sex
can be confused with sexual behavior, the term same-
gender sexual behavior was used in the previous para-
graph even though it refers to a sexual interaction be-
tween two people of the same biological sex. The fol-
lowing example from the Publication Manual also illus-
trates the use of gender in a context where the term sex
might be confusing: “In accounting for attitudes toward
the bill, sexual orientation rather than gender accounted
for most of the variance. Most gay men and lesbians were
for the proposal; most heterosexual men and women were
against it (APA, 2001, p. 63).”
Disabilities. When referring to individuals with dis-
abilities, maintain their integrity as individuals and human
beings by avoiding language that equates them with their
conditions. Don’t use nouns such as neurotics, schizo-
phrenics, manic-depressives, the mentally retarded, or
even the disabled. Also avoid terms that imply victimiza-
tion (e.g., “suffers from schizophrenia,” AIDS victim)
or that can be interpreted as a slur (e.g., cripple). In gen-
eral, the preferred forms of description are “person with
_______” or “person living with ______” or “person who
has________.” Challenged and special are often consid-
ered euphemistic and should be used only if preferred by
those who participate in your study.
There is one exception to these guidelines: the Deaf.
(Note the capital “D.”) Although some individuals with
reduced hearing may find the term hearing impaired ac-
ceptable, many Deaf individuals do not. They regard
themselves as members of a distinctive linguistic culture
that communicates in a manual (sign) language. Accord-
ingly, they take pride in referring to themselves as Deaf
and do not regard themselves as impaired or disabled. Do
not use the terms hard of hearing or deaf mute to describe
them. If your study involved Deaf participants, ask them
how they prefer to be described.
Common Errors of Grammar and Usage
The following errors seem to me to be the most fre-
quent in journal writing (listed alphabetically):
Compared with versus Compared to. Similar orders
of things are compared with one another; different orders
of things are compared to one another: “Let me not com-
pare thee with previous lovers I have had; rather, let me
compare thee to a summer’s day.” Mischel’s articles are
often compared with Bandura’s articles; Bem’s articles
are often compared to Mozart’s sonatas.”
Data. The word data is plural: “Analyze those data
thoroughly.”
Different from versus Different than. The first is cor-
rect, the second, incorrect (although, alas for us purists,
very common and gaining respectability). The confusion
arises because than correctly follows comparative adjec-
tives. Thus you are correct to suppose that life is more
than psychology, that living a good life is harder in many
respects than writing a good article, and that living well
requires broader skills than does writing well. Just re-
member that life is different from psychology, that living
a good life is different in many respects from writing a
good article, and that living well requires skills different
from those required for writing well.
Since versus Because. Since means “after that.” It
should not be used as a substitute for because if there is
any ambiguity of interpretation. Wrong (but at least not
ambiguous): “Since the study of motivation is a high and
hazardous undertaking, I wish fewer people would med-
dle with it.” Better: “Because the study of motivation is a
high and hazardous undertaking, I wish fewer people
would meddle with it.” Ambiguous: “Since I read Mon-
taigne, I have been tempted to abandon the study of moti-
vation.” This last case is correct if the writer is using since
in the temporal sense: “Ever since reading Montaigne, I
have been tempted ...” It is incorrect if the writer means
because.
That versus Which. That clauses (called restrictive)
are essential to the meaning of the sentence; which
clauses (called nonrestrictive) merely add additional in-
formation. The following example illustrates the correct
use of both words: “Dissonance theory, which has re-
ceived major attention, is one of the theories that postu-
lates a motivational process. Thus, if a person holds two
cognitions that are inconsistent...” Most which’s in jour-
nal writing are incorrect. You should go on a which hunt
in your own manuscripts and turn most of them into
that’s.
While versus Although, But, Whereas. While means
“at the same time” and in most cases cannot substitute for
these other words. Wrong: “While inferential statistics are
important, descriptive statistics are the heart of your nar-
rative.” Right: Although inferential statistics are impor-
tant, descriptive statistics are the heart of your narrative.”
Or, “Inferential statistics are important, but descriptive
statistics are the heart of your narrative.” Wrong: While I
like personality traits, Mischel prefers a social learning
approach.” Right: Whereas I like personality traits,
Mischel prefers a social learning approach.” On the other
hand, the following usage is correct: While I like person-
ality traits, I find merit in Mischel’s social learning ap-
proach.” This can be seen by substituting “at the same
time” for “while”: “I like personality traits; at the same
time, I find merit in Mischel’s social learning approach.”
Publishing Your Article
Long ago and far away, a journal editor allegedly ac-
cepted a manuscript that required no revisions. I believe
the author was William James. In other words, if your
article is provisionally accepted for publication “pending
revisions in accord with the reviewers’ comments,” you
should be deliriously happy. Publication is now virtually
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 17
under your control. If your article is rejected but you are
invited to resubmit a revised version, you should still be
happy—if not deliriously so—because you still have a
reasonable shot at getting it published.
But this is the point at which many authors give up.
As one former editor noted,
in my experience as an associate editor, I thought a
good deal of variance in predicting eventual publica-
tion came from this phase of the process. Authors are
often discouraged by negative feedback and miss the
essential positive fact that they have been asked to re-
vise! They may never resubmit at all, or may let an in-
ordinate amount of time pass before they do (during
which editors and reviewers become unavailable, lose
the thread of the project, and so forth). An opposite
problem is that some authors become defensive and
combative, and refuse to make needed changes for no
reason.
So do not give up at this point. Feel free to complain
to your colleagues or rail at your poodle because the stu-
pid reviewers failed to read your manuscript correctly.
But then turn to the task of revising your manuscript with
a dispassionate, problem-solving approach. First, pay spe-
cial attention to criticisms or suggestions made by more
than one reviewer or highlighted by the editor in the cover
letter. These must be addressed in your revision—even if
not in exactly the way the editor or reviewers suggest.
Second, look carefully at each of the reviewers’ mis-
readings. I argued earlier that whenever readers of a
manuscript find something unclear, they are right; by
definition, the writing is unclear. The problem is that
readers themselves do not always recognize or identify
the unclarities explicitly. Instead, they misunderstand
what you have written and then make a criticism or offer
a suggestion that makes no sense. In other words, you
should also interpret reviewers’ misreadings as signals
that your writing is unclear.
Think of your manuscript as a pilot experiment in
which the pilot participants (reviewers) did not under-
stand the instructions you gave them. Analyze the reasons
for their misunderstanding and then rewrite the problem-
atic sections so that subsequent readers will not be simi-
larly misled. Compared with the average journal reader,
reviewers are almost always more knowledgeable about
your topic, more experienced in writing manuscripts
themselves, and more conscientious about reading your
article. If they did not understand, neither will that aver-
age reader.
Third, when you send in your revised manuscript, tell
the editor in a cover letter how you have responded to
each of the criticisms or suggestions made by the review-
ers. If you have decided not to adopt a particular sugges-
tion, state your reasons, perhaps pointing out how you
remedied the problem in some alternative way.
Here are some fictitious examples of cover-letter re-
sponses that also illustrate ways of responding to certain
kinds of criticisms and suggestions within the revision
itself.
1. Wrong: “I have left the section on the animal
studies unchanged. If Reviewers A and C can’t even agree
on whether the animal studies are relevant, I must be do-
ing something right.”
Right: “You will recall that Reviewer A thought
that the animal studies should be described more fully,
whereas Reviewer C thought they should be omitted. A
biopsychologist in my department agreed with Reviewer
C that the animal studies are not really valid analogs of
the human studies. So I have dropped them from the text
but cited Snarkle’s review of them in an explanatory foot-
note on page 26.”
2. Wrong: “Reviewer A is obviously Deborah Har-
din, who has never liked me or my work. If she really
thinks that behavioral principles solve all the problems of
obsessive-compulsive disorders, then let her write her
own article. Mine is about the cognitive processes in-
volved.”
Right: “As the critical remarks by Reviewer A in-
dicate, this is a contentious area, with different theorists
staking out strong positions. Apparently I did not make it
clear that my article was intended only to cover the cog-
nitive processes involved in obsessive-compulsive disor-
ders and not to engage the debate between cognitive and
behavioral approaches. To clarify this, I have now in-
cluded the word ‘cognitive’ in both the title and abstract,
taken note of the debate in my introduction, and stated
explicitly that the article will not undertake a comparative
review of the two approaches. I hope this is satisfactory.”
3. Right: “You will recall that two of the reviewers
questioned the validity of the analysis of variance, with
Reviewer B suggesting that I use multiple regression in-
stead. I agree with their reservations regarding the
ANOVA but believe that a multiple regression analysis is
equally problematic because it makes the same assump-
tions about the underlying distributions. So, I have re-
tained the ANOVA, but summarized the results of a non-
parametric analysis, which yields the same conclusions. If
you think it preferable, I could simply substitute this non-
parametric analysis for the original ANOVA, although it
will be less familiar to the journal’s readers.”
Above all, remember that the editor is your ally in
trying to shape a manuscript that will be a credit both to
you and the journal. So, cooperate in the effort to turn
your sow’s ear into a vinyl purse. Be civil and make nice.
You may not live longer, but you will publish more.
References
American Psychological Association. (2001). Publication
manual of the American Psychological Association
(5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Writing the Empirical Journal Article 18
Bem, D. J. (1995) Writing a review article for Psycho-
logical Bulletin. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 172-
177.
Bem, D. J. & Allen, A. (1974). Predicting some of the
people some of the time: The search for cross-
situational consistencies in behavior. Psychological
Review, 81, 506-520.
Bem, S. L. (1985). Androgyny and gender schema theory:
A conceptual and empirical integration. In T. B. Son-
deregger (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation
1984: The psychology of gender. Lincoln: University
of Nebraska Press.
Festinger, L. A. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Festinger, L. & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive conse-
quences of forced compliance. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 58, 203-210.
Martyna, W. (1978). What does “He” mean? Journal of
Communication, 28, 131-138.
de Montaigne, M. (1943). Of the inconsistency of our
actions. In D. M. Frame (Trans.), Selected essays,
translated and with introduction and notes by Donald
M. Frame (pp. 119-126). Roslyn, NY: Walter J.
Black. (Original work published 1580)
Strunk, W., Jr., & White, E. B. (2000). The elements of
style (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.